Abstract
Background: The use of pain neuroscience education (PNE) has been shown to be effective in reducing pain, improving function and lowering fear and catastrophisation. Pain neuroscience education utilises various stories and metaphors to help patients reconceptualise their pain experience. To date no individualised study has looked at which stories and metaphors may be the most effective in achieving the positive outcomes found with the use of PNE.
Objectives: This study examined patient responses to the usefulness of the various stories and metaphors used during PNE for patients who underwent surgery for lumbar radiculopathy.
Method: Twenty-seven participants who received preoperative PNE from a previous randomised control trial (RCT) were surveyed 1-year post-education utilising a 5-point Likert scale (0 – ‘do not remember’, 4 – ‘very helpful’) on the usefulness of the various stories and metaphors used during the PNE session. Participant demographics and outcomes data (pain intensity, function and pain knowledge) were utilised from the previous RCT for analysis and correlations.
Results: Nineteen surveys were returned for a response rate of 70%. No story or metaphor mean was below 2 – ‘neutral’, lowest mean at 2.53; 6 of the 11 stories or metaphors scored a mean above 3 – ‘helpful’.
Conclusion: No individual story or metaphor stood out as being predominately important in being helpful in the recovery process through the use of PNE.
Clinical implications: The overall messages of reconceptualising pain during PNE may be more important than any individual story or metaphor.
Keywords: pain neuroscience education; metaphors; lumbar radiculopathy surgery; physiotherapy; survey; chronic pain.
Introduction
Traditional biomedical education for pain-related musculoskeletal conditions focuses on a structural pathological model as a means of explaining why someone is going through a pain experience. There is evidence to show that biomedical education, which often produces potential negative expectations through verbal suggestions, may influence pain perception in a negative way (Blasini et al. 2017). Pain neuroscience education (PNE) is an educational strategy within the biopsychosocial model of care for individuals with pain-related musculoskeletal conditions. Pain neuroscience education incorporates the multidimensionality of a pain experience and helps patients reconceptualise pain through understanding the multiple neurophysiological, neurobiological, sociological and physical components that may be involved in their individual pain experience (Moseley 2007; Moseley & Butler 2015). Pain neuroscience education utilises various metaphors and analogies to explain the neurophysiological processes of pain occurring within the patient, along with the various other multidimensional aspects that may contribute to the patient’s pain experience.
Pain neuroscience education has shown promise as an effective educational strategy adjunct to a comprehensive multimodal rehabilitation programme (Louw et al. 2016). Various studies have shown, through the use of PNE, reductions in pain (Moseley 2002, 2003a), improved function (Van Oosterwijck et al. 2013; Vibe Fersum et al. 2013), decreased fear of movement (Téllez-García et al. 2014) and less catastrophising (Meeus et al. 2010).
While the effectiveness of the PNE approach has shown promise based on the overall concept of reconceptualising pain, the effectiveness of individual metaphors and analogies to accomplish this within a PNE session is less well known.
Even with the understanding that pain neurobiology and neurophysiology explained to patients via stories, metaphors and pictures are helpful, there is currently no evidence to determine if the total overall educational exposure (all of the stories) or single stories are most helpful in causing a conceptual shift in a person struggling with pain. Because of increasing demands on healthcare providers in regard to time management, resources distribution and healthcare costs (Orszag & Ellis 2007), it may be beneficial to explore this question, as a means of balancing cost-effectiveness of PNE delivery (Moseley 2003b). The aim of this study was to determine, from a patient’s perspective, which metaphors they found most helpful in developing a greater understanding of their pain experience 1 year after they had received a PNE session before lumbar surgery (LS) for radiculopathy.
Research methods and design
Design
Our study was a non-experimental cross-sectional descriptive survey of participants after PNE. It is one of the follow-up studies resulting from a multi-centre randomised controlled trial (RCT) of preoperative PNE before LS for radiculopathy, which has been published elsewhere (Louw et al. 2014).
Participants
A total of 27 participants who completed the 1-year follow-up from the original RCT treatment group that received PNE were eligible for our study. Original inclusion criteria were the following: (1) scheduled for LS for radiculopathy; (2) willingness to comply with the predetermined follow-ups and (3) willingness to complete postoperative questionnaires at designated time intervals. Exclusion criteria were the following: (1) age younger than 18 years or older than 65 years; (2) not being proficient in reading or comprehending the English language; (3) scheduled for LS involving instrumentation (e.g. spinal fusion, arthroplasty); (4) participation in a formal back school or multidisciplinary pain management programme; (5) undergoing LS for a condition other than lumbar radiculopathy; (6) the presence of chronic-pain-related conditions (e.g. fibromyalgia, chronic fatigue syndrome) or (7) symptoms of cord compression.
Preoperative pain neuroscience education
The development and content of the preoperative PNE session has been published elsewhere (Louw et al. 2013). Table 1 provides information about the content and the metaphors used, and Figures 1 and 2 demonstrate some of the pictures used with the metaphors to describe each aspect of the preoperative PNE programme. The PNE was provided by participating physiotherapists who had been trained and tested in the PNE programme. The PNE occurred in a one-on-one verbal format, with the use of pictures, examples, metaphors and drawings as needed. This was done in a conversational and personal approach rather than a lecture format. To ensure a standardised PNE programme, a systematic checklist was developed. The educational sessions averaged 30 min. Patients were additionally provided with a preoperative PNE booklet summarising the educational content of the preoperative PNE session, including pictures, examples and metaphors. Patients were asked to read the PNE booklet at least once before and once after their surgery.
|
FIGURE 1: Example of picture to explain ‘extra-sensitive alarm’. |
|
|
FIGURE 2: Example of picture to explain ‘hospital experiences’. |
|
TABLE 1: Description of the metaphors and target topics used in the preoperative pain neuroscience education. |
Survey development
A survey was developed, based on the aims of the study and previous survey studies used by the research team in developing PNE programmes for low back pain and spine surgery (Louw et al. 2012, 2015b). The survey asked patients to recall 11 key elements of the PNE and indicate how helpful each story or example was in their recovery. The replies were scored using a Likert scale: 0 = do not remember, 1 = not very helpful, 2 = neutral, 3 = helpful and 4 = very helpful. In addition, each patient was asked to specifically write down which single story or example helped most, and there was space for general comments or feedback on the preoperative PNE information. To establish face and content validity, the draft survey was sent to a panel of 10 national and international experts in the field of PNE (Louw et al. 2012; Powell 2003). Experts were asked to provide feedback on the content and completion of the PNE survey and return comments in 3 weeks. A reminder email was sent to panelists if they had not completed the accompanying checklist for the survey after 3 weeks. The survey was deemed ready for the next phase when 70% agreement was obtained by the expert panel, following expert review, minor grammatical, punctuation and spacing changes were made. A pilot study comprising a convenience sample of five patients with chronic pain, from a clinic that specialises in treating chronic pain, was used to review the content. These patients had received similar PNE and were asked to review the content, the ease of completion and the time it took to complete the questionnaire (10 min). Upon further minor editing, the final survey was deemed ready for distribution.
Survey distribution
After the 1-year follow-up for the RCT (Louw et al. 2014), each eligible participant was sent a letter, thanking them for their participation in the original RCT and asking them to complete the newly designed PNE metaphor survey. Participants were provided with a stamped, return addressed envelope for the completed survey. Participants who did not reply to the metaphor survey were contacted telephonically after 1 month and asked to please complete the survey.
Data analysis
Demographic data and the outcomes of the survey data were extracted and entered into an Excel spreadsheet with conversion to SPSS version 24 (IBM Corporation) for full analysis. Descriptive statistics such as means, counts and percentages were used to describe the participants. Ranges, mode and frequency of each metaphor were calculated from survey data.
Ethical considerations
The study was approved by the Health Research Ethics Committee at Stellenbosch University (Ethics Reference #: N09/09/247).
Results
Participants
Nineteen of the 27 participants (response rate = 70.3%) completed the PNE metaphor survey. Participants completing the survey had a mean age of 51.3 years and 11 were female (58%). Table 2 provides further demographic information of the participants completing the survey.
TABLE 2: Participant demographics n = 19. |
Pain metaphors
Table 3 shows the range, mode, mean and standard deviations for each question as rated by each participant.
TABLE 3: Individual metaphor information. |
The overall ranking (mean of the means) of the pain metaphors was 2.94 on the 0–4-point Likert scale. The highest ranking (mean score) metaphors in order of importance were (1) the general concept of pain being normal after surgery (3.21), (2) extra-sensitive alarm system (3.16), (3) the body’s living alarm system (3.11), (4) surgery experience influencing nerve sensitivity (3.11) and (5) nosy neighbours (2.89) (Figure 3).
|
FIGURE 3: Rating of the pain neuroscience education metaphors (0 = cannot remember; 4 = very helpful). |
|
One participant scored ‘dry and wet brain’ and ‘yellow flags’ as 0 – do not remember. Another participant scored ‘hurt does not equal harm’ as 1 – not very helpful. All of the other metaphor stories scored between 2 – neutral and 4 – very helpful on the Likert scale. The median score for all the metaphors was 3 – helpful. The mode for each metaphor ranked 3 – helpful, except for ‘nosy neighbours’ where the mode was 2 – neutral and ‘dry and wet brain’ which tied between 2 – neutral and 3 – helpful with the participants. The overall combined themes centred around two main issues: ‘easy to understand’ and ‘knew what to expect after surgery’.
Discussion
This is the first study to investigate, from a patient’s perspective, if specific metaphors associated with PNE are more helpful than others. The results from this study indicate no one metaphor seems to be superior to other metaphors for people undergoing PNE prior to LS for radiculopathy at the 1-year recovery follow-up period. Patients are interested in learning more about pain (Louw, Louw & Crous 2009; Moseley 2003b). The results from this study show that patients value the metaphors and stories shared as part of the PNE. Only one participant had a ‘not helpful’ grading of the ‘hurt does not equal harm’ metaphor and another scored ‘dry and wet brain’ and ‘yellow flags’ as ‘do not remember’. The rest of the participants scored the metaphors between helpful and very helpful. These results, similar to earlier PNE research that shows patients are able to take on ‘complex’ pain issues, which are taught via metaphors, examples and images (Moseley 2003b). No single metaphor was found to be better than the others in our study. However, each individual story together seemingly created an overall story, which enabled the patient to develop a greater understanding of pain and their pain experience as a whole.
The layout of the material in our study was similar to the study by Gallagher et al., who had patients read a book of metaphors associated with pain science, covering 11 different sections, and not one specific story (Gallagher, Mcauley & Moseley 2013). The mentioned study showed that the combination of metaphors tested in the book provided a significant shift in patients’ pain catastrophisation and pain knowledge.
The PNE programme tested in this trial was a culmination of more than a half-dozen studies to develop the PNE programme, with the intent to address all (or most) of the pertinent information patients may need to know after undergoing lumbar radiculopathy surgery (Louw et al. 2009, 2012, 2013). The results would indicate the PNE programme attained this goal, given all the metaphors were ranked as important and no one metaphor was found to be most important.
From the patients’ perspective, the overall theme of ‘pain after surgery is expected and normal’ carried some weight, both in being ranked highest and in the summary of the general themes. This may be specific to this patient population as it has been shown that patients have a mismatch in regard to their expectations and experiences when it comes to LS (Toyone et al. 2005). Various studies have shown that patients expect to be ‘pain-free’ after LS, which may not be biologically plausible (Keulers et al. 2008; Mancuso et al. 2014; Toyone et al. 2005). It is argued that this mismatch may, indeed, increase the potential for persistent pain (Keulers et al. 2008; Louw et al. 2012). Louw et al. 2009 interviewed patients who underwent LS for radiculopathy 4 weeks after surgery and found 50% of the patients had increased fear that their pain would increase, rather than subside (Louw et al. 2009). Fear has been strongly correlated to increased pain experiences (Pincus et al. 2006; Poiraudeau et al. 2006; Simon, Stryker & George 2011; Vlaeyen & Linton 2000). In fact, a population-based survey of LS indicates that the general population expects a long recovery after LS (Landers et al. 2014). It is within this framework that preoperative PNE is so important to be applicable to the clinical situation and pain picture of the patient.
Pain is a normal human experience (Moseley 2007; Woolf 2000). It has been shown that pain is common following LS (Oosterhuis et al. 2014; Ostelo et al. 2003a, 2003b, 2004), and patients need to be prepared for this, understand it and develop healthy cognitions. In our study, for example, 60% of the patients underwent surgery because of a primary complaint of pain and when completing the survey 1 year after surgery, mean low back pain was still 4.1/10 (Numeric rating scale [NRS]). Traditional biomedical pain education model equates pain with injury, while PNE helps patients develop an understanding of a hypervigilant nervous system (Louw et al. 2013). Even though pain is experienced, it is reconceptualised as sensitisation versus injury. It is believed that this reconceptualisation then, in turn, normalises pain catastrophisation and fear avoidance, which supports the current evidence for PNE addressing these psychosocial issues (Gallagher et al. 2013; Louw et al. 2011).
All of the stories contained in the preoperative PNE programme centred around this theme of sensitisation and normality of pain, which further strengthens the argument why the sum of all the metaphors was seen as beneficial (Louw 2012), versus a single metaphor. In the development of the preoperative PNE programme, the authors conducted a case series of immediate changes of patients learning these metaphors and showed a more realistic expectation of pain after surgery (Louw, Diener & Puentedura 2015a), which agrees with the idea that it is the sum of all the metaphors.
While this is an initial exploratory survey investigating which PNE metaphors may be most effective and significant and follow-up research is needed, it may provide some clinical insights in the delivery of PNE. Our study showed that no one metaphor was more important than another. Therefore, clinicians do not have to feel tied into using a specific set protocol to deliver PNE. Considering the multidimensionality of pain and the heterogeneous nature of each individual in pain, this makes sense that the delivery of PNE in a clinical setting is probably best delivered in a heterogeneous fashion based on the patient in front of the clinician.
The most effective dosage (time spent) on NPE has not been established, and may vary hugely between acute and persistent pain situations, and between low and high sensitisation. The systematic review by Louw et al. (2011), and their follow-up systematic review 5 years later (Louw et al. 2016), did not inform our understanding as to which portions within the content of PNE might be the most effective and what time needs to be spent on PNE. Patients with low distress, and minimum psychosocial contributors to pain, may need a few specific metaphors. Given the complexity and multi-faceted aspects associated with persistent pain, including peripheral and central sensitisation, ion channel expression, fear avoidance, fear and anxiety, (Moseley 2007; Nijs et al. 2013), it is not surprising to find that a single or a few stories may in fact be inadequate to address all or most of the multidimensional aspects of pain that people may face. Reducing a multidimensional pain experience down to a few metaphors to describe and provide meaning to someone’s pain experience may not be adequate. A strong therapeutic alliance and clinical communication emphasising ‘listening’ to the patient (Diener, Kargela & Louw 2016) will enhance this process, as it may enable the therapist to use metaphors specifically applicable to the patient’s pain context. Another important clinical implication was that there appeared to be value in all of the PNE stories being tied together towards an overarching theme of why they had ongoing pain.
Therefore, which individual metaphors the clinician utilises may not be as important as long as they all tie into a central understanding of why the patient still hurts. As the patient triangulates information from the various stories and metaphors as part of the PNE process, it may be important that there are no contradictory messages from the healthcare provider and there is congruency in the message.
Limitations
This study contains various limitations. The sample size is a small sample of convenience with no a priori determination of sample size, so it may be underpowered. The response rate of less than 100% leads to potential selection bias that may have occurred. It is also specific to patients who are recovering from lumbar radiculopathy surgery, all influencing the ability to generalise the findings of our study. The study design of selecting recall of the intervention at 1-year follow-up does not give an indication of metaphor preferences of participants earlier in the recovery process from surgery. Another limitation is the singularity of the mode and style of delivery within this specific study (Louw et al. 2014). While there are similarities and differences to the various PNE modes of delivery (Louw et al. 2016), no current exploratory study has looked at how the potential differences may affect outcomes differently. There is further research needed to explore how these differences may or may not affect outcomes, and until that is completed, caution should be taken in interpreting these results too broadly.
Conclusion
No single metaphor or story was superior to other metaphors in the PNE process to help individuals in their recovery after lumbar radiculopathy surgery. Multiple pain metaphors, combined into one single story, may be important from a patient’s perspective, in helping them understand their pain experience.
Acknowledgements
The authors acknowledge the participants for their time and insight for completing the surveys.
Competing interests
Adriaan Louw, Emilio J. Puentedura, Ina Diener, and Kory J. Zimney have published books on pain neuroscience education and receive royalties from them.
Authors’ contributions
A.L. was the project leader; I.D. and E.J.P. assisted in project design; K.J.Z. provided statistical analysis and T.C. was involved in data analysis and IRB submission through Southwest Baptist University. A.L., I.D., E.J.P., K.J.Z. and T.C. were all involved in manuscript writing and preparation.
Funding
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.
Data availability statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.
Disclaimer
The views expressed in this article are their own and not an official position of the institution or funder.
References
Blasini, M., Corsi, N., Klinger, R. & Colloca, L., 2017, ‘Nocebo and pain: An overview of the psychoneurobiological mechanisms’, Pain Reports 2(2), e585. https://doi.org/10.1097/PR9.0000000000000585
Diener, I., Kargela, M. & Louw, A., 2016, ‘Listening is therapy: Patient interviewing from a pain science perspective’, Physiotherapy Theory and Practice 32(5), 356–367. https://doi.org/10.1080/09593985.2016.1194648
Gallagher, L., Mcauley, J. & Moseley, G.L., 2013, ‘A randomized-controlled trial of using a book of metaphors to reconceptualize pain and decrease catastrophizing in people with chronic pain’, The Clinical Journal of Pain 29(1), 20–25. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182465cf7
Keulers, B.J., Scheltinga, M.R., Houterman, S., Van Der Wilt, G.J. & Spauwen, P.H., 2008, ‘Surgeons underestimate their patients’ desire for preoperative information’, World Journal of Surgery 32(6), 964–970. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00268-008-9581-1
Landers, M.R., Puentedura, E., Louw, A., Mccauley, A., Rasmussen, Z. & Bungum, T., 2014, ‘A population-based survey of lumbar surgery beliefs in the United States’, Orthopaedic Nursing/National Association of Orthopaedic Nurses 33(4), 207–216. https://doi.org/10.1097/NOR.0000000000000064
Louw, A., 2012, Your nerves are having back surgery, OPTP, Minneapolis, MN.
Louw, A., Butler, D. S., Diener, I. & Puentedura, E.J., 2012, ‘Preoperative education for lumbar radiculopathy: A survey of US spine surgeons’, International Journal of Spine Surgery 6, 130–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsp.2012.03.001
Louw, A., Butler, D.S., Diener, I. & Puentedura, E.J., 2013, ‘Development of a preoperative neuroscience educational program for patients with lumbar radiculopathy’, American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation/Association of Academic Physiatrists 92(5), 446–452. https://doi.org/10.1097/PHM.0b013e3182876aa4
Louw, A., Diener, I., Butler, D. & Puentedura, E., 2011, ‘The effect of neuroscience education on pain, disability, anxiety, and stress in chronic musculoskeletal pain’,. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 92(12), 2041–2056. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2011.07.198
Louw, A., Diener, I., Landers, M.R. & Puentedura, E.J., 2014, ‘Preoperative pain neuroscience education for lumbar radiculopathy: A multicenter randomized controlled trial with 1-year follow-up’, Spine 39(18), 1449–1457. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0000000000000444
Louw, A., Diener, I. & Puentedura, E.J., 2015a, ‘The short term effects of preoperative neuroscience education for lumbar radiculopathy: A case series’, International Journal of Spine Surgery 9, 11. https://doi.org/10.14444/2011
Louw, A., Louw, Q. & Crous, L.C.C., 2009, ‘Preoperative education for lumbar surgery for radiculopathy’, South African Journal of Physiotherapy 65(2), 3–8. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajp.v65i2.244
Louw, A., Puentedura, E.J. & Zimney, K., 2015b, ‘A clinical contrast: Physical therapists with low back pain treating patients with low back pain’, Physiotherapy Theory and Practice 31(8), 562–567. https://doi.org/10.3109/09593985.2015.1062944
Louw, A., Zimney, K., Puentedura, E.J. & Diener, I., 2016, ‘The efficacy of therapeutic neuroscience education on musculoskeletal pain – A systematic review of the literature’, Physiotherapy Theory and Practice 32(5), 332–355. https://doi.org/10.1080/09593985.2016.1194646
Mancuso, C.A., Duculan, R., Stal, M. & Girardi, F.P., 2014, ‘Patients’ expectations of cervical spine surgery’, Spine 39(14), 1157–1162. https://doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0000000000000349
Meeus, M., Nijs, J., Van Oosterwijck, J., Van Alsenoy, V. & Truijen, S., 2010, ‘Pain physiology education improves pain beliefs in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome compared with pacing and self-management education: A double-blind randomized controlled trial’, Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 91(8), 1153–1159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2010.04.020
Moseley, G., 2002, ‘Combined physiotherapy and education is efficacious for chronic low back pain’, Australian Journal of Physiotherapy 48(4), 297–302. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0004-9514(14)60169-0
Moseley, G.L., 2003a, ‘Joining forces – Combining cognitiion – Targeted motor control training with group or individual pain physiology education: A successful tretment for chronic low back pain’, Journal of Manual & Manipulative Therapy 11(2), 88–94. https://doi.org/10.1179/106698103790826383
Moseley, G.L., 2007, ‘Reconceptualising pain according to modern pain science’, Physical Therapy Reviews 12(3), 169–178. https://doi.org/10.1179/108331907X223010
Moseley, G.L. & Butler, D.S., 2015, ‘Fifteen years of explaining pain: The past, present, and future’, The Journal of Pain 16(9), 807–813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2015.05.005
Moseley, L., 2003b, ‘Unraveling the barriers to reconceptualization of the problem in chronic pain: The actual and perceived ability of patients and health professionals to understand the neurophysiology’, The Journal of Pain 4(4), 184–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1526-5900(03)00488-7
Nijs, J., Roussel, N., Paul Van Wilgen, C., Koke, A. & Smeets, R., 2013, ‘Thinking beyond muscles and joints: Therapists’ and patients’ attitudes and beliefs regarding chronic musculoskeletal pain are key to applying effective treatment’, Manual Therapy 18(2), 96–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.math.2012.11.001
Oosterhuis, T., Costa, L.O., Maher, C.G., De Vet, H.C., Van Tulder, M.W. & Ostelo, R.W., 2014, ‘Rehabilitation after lumbar disc surgery’, The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 3, CD003007. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD003007.pub3
Orszag, P.R. & Ellis, P., 2007, ‘The challenge of rising health care costs – A view from the Congressional Budget Office’, The New England Journal of Medicine 357(18), 1793–1795. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp078190
Ostelo, R.W., Costa, L.O., Maher, C.G., De Vet, H.C. & Van Tulder, M.W., 2008, ‘Rehabilitation after lumbar disc surgery’, The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 4, CD003007.
Ostelo, R.W., De Vet, H.C., Berfelo, M.W., Kerckhoffs, M.R., Vlaeyen, J.W., Wolters, P.M. et al., 2003a, ‘Effectiveness of behavioral graded activity after first-time lumbar disc surgery: Short term results of a randomized controlled trial’, European Spine Journal 12(6), 637–644. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-003-0560-9
Ostelo, R.W., De Vet, H.C., Vlaeyen, J.W., Kerckhoffs, M.R., Berfelo, W.M., Wolters, P.M. et al., 2003b, ‘Behavioral graded activity following first-time lumbar disc surgery: 1-year results of a randomized clinical trial’, Spine 28(16), 1757–1765. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.BRS.0000083317.62258.E6
Pincus, T., Vogel, S., Burton, A.K., Santos, R. & Field, A.P., 2006, ‘Fear avoidance and prognosis in back pain: A systematic review and synthesis of current evidence’, Arthritis & Rheumatology 54(12), 3999–4010. https://doi.org/10.1002/art.22273
Poiraudeau, S., Rannou, F., Baron, G., Le Henanff, A., Coudeyre, E., Rozenberg, S. et al., 2006, ‘Fear-avoidance beliefs about back pain in patients with subacute low back pain’, Pain 124(3), 305–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.019
Powell, C., 2003, ‘The Delphi technique: Myths and realities’, Journal of Advanced Nursing 41(4), 376–382. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.2003.02537.x
Simon, C.B., Stryker, S.E. & George, S.Z., 2011, ‘Comparison of work-related fear-avoidance beliefs across different anatomical locations with musculoskeletal pain’, Journal of Pain Research 4, 253–262. https://doi.org/10.2147/JPR.S22457
Téllez-García, M., De-La-Llave-Rincón, A.I., Salom-Moreno, J., Palacios-Ceña, M., Ortega-Santiago, R. & Fernández-De-Las-Peñas, C., 2014, ‘Neuroscience education in addition to trigger point dry needling for the management of patients with mechanical chronic low back pain: A preliminary clinical trial’, Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies 19(3), 464–472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbmt.2014.11.012
Toyone, T., Tanaka, T., Kato, D., Kaneyama, R. & Otsuka, M., 2005, ‘Patients’ expectations and satisfaction in lumbar spine surgery’, Spine 30(23), 2689–2694. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.brs.0000187876.14304.15
Van Oosterwijck, J., Meeus, M., Paul, L., De Schryver, M., Pascal, A., Lambrecht, L. et al., 2013, ‘Pain physiology education improves health status and endogenous pain inhibition in fibromyalgia: A double-blind randomized controlled trial’, The Clinical Journal of Pain 29(10), 873–882. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e31827c7a7d
Vibe Fersum, K., O’sullivan, P., Skouen, J., Smith, A. & Kvåle, A., 2013, ‘Efficacy of classification-based cognitive functional therapy in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain: A randomized controlled trial’, European Journal of Pain 17(6), 916–928. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00252.x
Vlaeyen, J.W.S. & Linton, S.J., 2000, ‘Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of the art’, Pain 85(3), 317–322. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0
Woolf, C.J., 2000, ‘Pain’, Neurobiology of Disease 7, 504–510. https://doi.org/10.1006/nbdi.2000.0342
|